JOURNAL BROWSE
Search
Advanced SearchSearch Tips
Relations Between Mothers' and Preschoolers' Use of Mental State Terms During Pretend Play and Preschoolers' Mental State Terms in Hypothetical Narratives
facebook(new window)  Pirnt(new window) E-mail(new window) Excel Download
  • Journal title : Korean Journal of Child Studies
  • Volume 37, Issue 2,  2016, pp.127-142
  • Publisher : Korean Association of Child Studies
  • DOI : 10.5723/kjcs.2016.37.2.127
 Title & Authors
Relations Between Mothers' and Preschoolers' Use of Mental State Terms During Pretend Play and Preschoolers' Mental State Terms in Hypothetical Narratives
Shin, Nana; Kim, Soyoung;
  PDF(new window)
 Abstract
Objective: This research examined mothers' and preschoolers' uses of mental state terms during pretend play and linked such use to children's independent mental state terms used in hypothetical narratives. Methods: Fifty four-year-olds and their mothers were engaged in pretend play and the preschoolers were asked to provide hypothetical narratives. Mothers' and preschoolers' mental state terms were analyzed in terms of types and frequencies. Results: During pretend play, the mothers and preschoolers used desire state terms most variously and frequently, followed by cognitive and emotion state terms. In the hypothetical narratives, the preschoolers used desire state terms most variously and frequently; however they talked about emotion state terms more variously and cognitive state terms more frequently. In addition, the mothers' mental state terms were correlated with the preschoolers' mental state terms during the pretend play, and the mothers' and preschoolers' uses of mental state terms during pretend play were related to the preschoolers' mental state terms in hypothetical narratives. Conclusion: Findings from this study highlight that, during the preschool period, a mothers' mental state language might foster her child's understanding of mental states in himself/herself and in others.
 Keywords
mental state term;pretend play;hypothetical narrative;
 Language
Korean
 Cited by
 References
1.
Adrian, J. E., Clemente, R. A., Villanueva, L., & Rieffe, C. (2005). Parent-child picture-book reading, mothers' mental state language and children's theory of mind. Journal of Child Language, 32(3), 673-686. doi:10.1017/S0305000905006963 crossref(new window)

2.
Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1002/1520-6807(199601)33:1<87::AIDPITS2310330105>3.0.CO;2-C

3.
Bettmann, J. E., & Lundahl, B. W. (2007). Tell me a story: A review of narrative assessments for preschoolers. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 24(5), 455-475. doi:10.1007/s10560-007-0095-8 crossref(new window)

4.
Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal states: The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 18(6), 906-921. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.18.6.906 crossref(new window)

5.
Bretherton, I., Oppenheim, D., Buchsbaum, H., & Emde, R. N. (1990). The MacArthur Story Stem Battery (MSSB). Unpublished Manual, University of Wisconsin-Madison, USA. doi: 10.13140/RG.2.1.2376.9442

6.
Brown, J. R., Donelan-McCall, N., & Dunn, J. (1996). Why talk about mental states? The significance of children's conversations with friends, siblings, and mothers. Child Development, 67(3), 836-849. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01767.x crossref(new window)

7.
Brown, J. R., & Dunn, J. (1991). "You can cry, mum": The social and developmental implications of talk about internal states. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9(2), 237-256. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.1991.tb00874.x crossref(new window)

8.
Carlson, S. M., Mandell, D. J., & Williams, L. (2004). Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 1105-1122. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1105 crossref(new window)

9.
Comparini, L., Douglas, E. M., & Perez, S. N. (2014). The development of social cognition: Preschoolers' use of mental state talk in peer conflicts. Early Education and Development, 25(7), 1083-1101. doi:10.1080/10409289.2014.896770 crossref(new window)

10.
Cutting, A, L., & Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language and family background: Individual differences and inter-relations. Child Development, 70(4), 853-865. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00061 crossref(new window)

11.
Denham, S. A., Blair, K. A., DeMulder, E., Levitas, J., Sawyer, K., Auerbach-Major, S., & Queenan, P. (2003). Preschool emotional competence: Pathway to social competence? Child Development, 74(1), 238-256. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00533 crossref(new window)

12.
Dunn, J., Bretherton, I., & Munn, P. (1987). Conversations about feeling states between mothers and their young children. Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 132-139. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.23.1.132 crossref(new window)

13.
Dyer, J. R., Shatz, M., & Wellman, H. M. (2000). Young children's storybooks as a source of mental state information. Cognitive Development, 15(1), 17-37. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(00)00017-4 crossref(new window)

14.
Flavell, J. H. (2000). Development of children's knowledge about the mental world. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24(1), 15-23. doi:10.1080/016502500383421 crossref(new window)

15.
Haight, W. L., & Miller, P. J. (1993). Pretending at home: Early development in a sociocultural context. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

16.
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1991). Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Development, 62(4), 782-796. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01569.x crossref(new window)

17.
Howe, N., Rinaldi, C. M., & Recchia, H. E. (2010). Patterns in mother-child internal state discourse across four contexts. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 56(1), 1-20. doi:10.1353/mpq.0.0042 crossref(new window)

18.
Hughes, C., & Dunn, J. (1997). "Pretend you didn't know": Preschooler's talk about mental states in pretend play. Cognitive Development, 12(4), 477-497. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(97)90019-8 crossref(new window)

19.
Hughes, C., & Dunn, J. (1998). Understanding mind and emotion: Longitudinal associations with mental-state talk between young friends. Developmental Psychology, 34(5), 1026-1037. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.34.5.1026 crossref(new window)

20.
Hughes, C., Marks, A., & Ensor, R. (2009). A longitudinal study of conflict and inner state talk in children's conversations with mothers and younger siblings. Social Development, 19(4), 822-837. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.00561.x crossref(new window)

21.
Hughes, C., Lecce, S., & Wilson, C. (2007). "Do you know what I want?" Preschoolers' talk about desires, thoughts and feelings in their conversations with sibs and friends. Cognition and Emotion, 21(2), 330-350. doi:10.1080/02699930600551691 crossref(new window)

22.
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 236-248. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.236 crossref(new window)

23.
Jenkins, J. M., & Astington, J. W. (2000). Theory of mind and social behavior: Causal models tested in a longitudinal study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46(2), 203-220. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/23093714

24.
Jenkins, J. M., Turrell, S. L., Kogushi, Y., Lollis, S., & Ross, H. S. (2003). A longitudinal investigation of the dynamics of mental state talk in families. Child Development, 74(3), 905-920. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00575 crossref(new window)

25.
Ji, E. J., & Jo, H. S. (2010). The relationship between mothers' mental state terms and children's. The Journal of Korea Open Association for Early Childhood Education, 15(1), 235-261.

26.
Kim, H. J. (2014). Mental-state talks of mothers with 2-year-olds in pretense/role-play and book reading contexts. Journal of Korean Child Care and Education, 10(2), 133-151. doi: 10.14698/jkcce.2014.10.2.133 crossref(new window)

27.
Kim, Y. S., & Lee, J. H. (2003). Symbolic levels of early pretense and representational thinking. Korean Journal of Child Studies, 24(5), 1-14. Retrieved from http://childstudies.org/journal/view.php?number=2599

28.
Kuebli, J., Butler, S., & Fivush, R. (1995). Mother-child talk about past emotions: Relations of maternal language and child gender over time. Cognition & Emotion, 9(2-3), 265-283. doi:10.1080/02699939508409011 crossref(new window)

29.
Laible, D. (2004). Mother-child discourse in two contexts: Links with child temperament, attachment security, and socioemotional competence. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 979-992. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.979 crossref(new window)

30.
Leaper, C., Anderson, K. J., & Sanders, P. (1998). Moderators of gender effects on parents' talk to their children: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 34(1), 3-27. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.34.1.3 crossref(new window)

31.
Lee, Y. J., & Lee, J. H. (2006). Young children's use of mental state terms in narratives according to the Levels of narrative development, theory of mind, and vocabulary. The Journal of Korea Open Association for Early Childhood Education, 11(1), 153-178.

32.
Lee, Y. J., Lee, J. S., & Shin, E. S. (2004). Differences in young children's levels of language functions of pretend play, vocabulary, and mental state term expressions for young children according to the theory of mind. Journal of Early Childhood Education, 24(4), 141-160.

33.
Lee, Y. J., & Oh, J. H. (2007). The effects of picturebook reading and emotional state discourse on young children’s development of fictional narratives and theory of mind. Journal of Early Childhood Education, 27(3), 245-271. crossref(new window)

34.
Miller, A. L., Fine, S. E., Gouley, K. K., Seifer, R., Dickstein, S., & Shields, A. (2006). Showing and telling about emotions: Interrelations between facets of emotional competence and associations with classroom adjustment in Head Start preschoolers. Cognition and Emotion, 20(8), 1170-1192. doi:10.1080/02699930500405691 crossref(new window)

35.
Moore, C., Furrow, D., Chiasson, L., & Patriquin, M. (1994). Developmental relationships between production and comprehension of mental terms. First Language, 14(40), 1-17. doi:10.1177/014272379401404001 crossref(new window)

36.
Nelson, K. (2005). Language pathways into the community of minds. In J. W. Astington & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 26-49). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

37.
Nielsen, M., & Dissanayake, C. (2000). An investigation of pretend play, mental state terms and false belief understanding: In search of a metarepresentational link. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18(4), 609-624. doi:10.1348/026151000165887 crossref(new window)

38.
Ontai, L. L., & Thompson, R. A. (2008). Attachment, parent-child discourse and theory-of-mind development. Social Development, 17(1), 47-60. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00414.x crossref(new window)

39.
Osorio, A., Meins, E., Martins, C., Martins, C. E., & Soares, I. (2012). Child and mother mental-state talk in shared pretense as predictors of children's social symbolic play abilities at age 3. Infant Behavior & Development, 35(4), 719-726. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.07.012 crossref(new window)

40.
Park, H. W., Kwak, K. J., & Park, K. B. (1996). Korean-Wechsler Primary Preschool Scale of Intelligence: K-WPPSI . Seoul: Special Education.

41.
Park, K. J., & Kwon, M. G. (2011). Young children's optimism in the relationship with mental state terms and story structure development. Science Review, 37, 131-146.

42.
Robinson, J. L. (2007). Story stem narratives with young children: Moving to clinical research and practice. Attachment & Human Development, 9(3), 179-185. doi:10.1080/14616730701453697 crossref(new window)

43.
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

44.
Rosnay, M. D., & Hughes, C. (2006). Conversation and theory of mind: Do children talk their way to socio-cognitive understanding? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(1), 7-37. doi:10.1348/026151005X82901 crossref(new window)

45.
Rudek, D. J., & Haden, C. A. (2005). Mothers' and preschoolers' mental state language during reminiscing over time. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 51(4), 523-549. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/23096100 crossref(new window)

46.
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children's and mother's mental state language and theoryof-mind understanding. Child Development, 73(3), 734-751. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00435 crossref(new window)

47.
Shatz, M., Wellman, H. M., & Silber, S. (1983). The acquisition of mental verbs: A systematic investigation of the first reference to mental state. Cognition, 14(3), 301-321. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(83)90008-2 crossref(new window)

48.
Song, Y. J. (2009). Children's mind-related-expressions in story making and their relations to theory of mind. The Journal of Korea Open Association for Early Childhood Education, 14(3), 425-446.

49.
Song, J., & Ohm, J. A. (2008). The relationship between the use of mental state terms and theory of mind in the sociodramatic play of 4 year-old children. Journal of Early Childhood Education, 28(2), 69-94.

50.
Symons, D. K. (2004). Mental state discourse, theory of mind, and the internalization of self–other understanding. Developmental Review, 24(2), 159-188. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2004.03.001 crossref(new window)

51.
Symons, D. K., Fossum, K. L. M., & Collins, T. B. (2006). A longitudinal study of belief and desire state discourse during mother-child play and later false belief understanding. Social Development, 15(4), 676-692. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2006.00364.x crossref(new window)

52.
Symons, D. K., Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V., Roche, J., & Doyle, E. (2005) Theory of mind and mental state discourse during book reading and story-telling tasks. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 81-102. doi:10.1348/026151004X21080 crossref(new window)

53.
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (1990). Language, play, and attention at one year. Infant Behavior and Development, 13(1), 85-98. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(90)90007-U crossref(new window)

54.
Taumoepeau, M., & Ruffman, T. (2006). Mother and infant talk about mental states relates to desire language and emotion understanding. Child Development, 77(2), 465-481. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00882.x crossref(new window)

55.
Taumoepeau, M., & Ruffman, T. (2008). Stepping stones to others' minds: Maternal talk relates to child mental state language and emotion understanding at 15, 24, and 33 months. Child Development, 79(2), 284-302. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01126.x crossref(new window)

56.
Thompson, R. A. (2006). The development of the person: Social understanding, relationships, conscience, self. In W. Damon (Series Ed.), Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 24-98). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. doi:10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0302

57.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

58.
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655-684. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00304 crossref(new window)

59.
Wellman, H. M., & Woolley, J. D. (1990). From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology. Cognition, 35(3), 245-275. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(90)90024-E crossref(new window)

60.
Ziv, M., Smadja, M. L., & Aram, D. (2013). Mothers’ mentalstate discourse with preschoolers during storybook reading and wordless storybook telling. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(1), 177-186. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.05.005 crossref(new window)