JOURNAL BROWSE
Search
Advanced SearchSearch Tips
Comparison of Stereoscopic Fusional Area between People with Good and Poor Stereo Acuity
facebook(new window)  Pirnt(new window) E-mail(new window) Excel Download
 Title & Authors
Comparison of Stereoscopic Fusional Area between People with Good and Poor Stereo Acuity
Kang, Hyungoo; Hong, Hyungki;
  PDF(new window)
 Abstract
Purpose: This study investigated differences in stereoscopic fusional area between those with good and poor stereo acuity in viewing stereoscopic displays. Methods: Stereo acuity of 39 participants (18 males and 21 females, ) was measured with the random dot stereo butterfly method. Participants with stereo-blindness were not included. Stereoscopic fusional area was measured using stereoscopic stimulus by varying the amount of horizontal disparity in a stereoscopic 3D TV. Participants were divided into two groups of good and poor stereo acuity. Criterion for good stereo acuity was determined as less than 60 arc seconds. Measurements arising from the participants were statistically analyzed. Results: 26 participants were measured to have good stereo acuity and 13 participants poor stereo acuity. In case of the stereoscopic stimulus farther than the fixation point, threshold of horizontal disparity for those with poor stereo acuity were measured to be smaller than the threshold for those with good stereo acuity, with a statistically significant difference. On the other hand, there was no statistically significant difference between the two groups, in case of the stereoscopic stimulus nearer to the fixation point. Conclusions: In viewing stereoscopic displays, the boundary of stereoscopic fusional area for the poor stereo acuity group was smaller than the boundary of good stereo acuity group only for the range behind the display. Hence, in viewing stereoscopic displays, participants with poor stereo acuity would have more difficulty perceiving the fused image at farther distances compared to participants with good stereo acuity.
 Keywords
Stereo acuity;Stereoscopic fusional area;Stereoscopic display;
 Language
English
 Cited by
 References
1.
Heron S, Lages M. Screening and sampling in studies of binocular vision. Vision Res. 2012;62:228-234. crossref(new window)

2.
Levi DM, McKee SP, Movshon JA. Visual deficits in anisometropia. Vision Res. 2011;51(1):48-57. crossref(new window)

3.
Jimenez JR, Olivares JL, Perez-Ocon F, del Barco LJ. Associated phoria in relation to stereopsis with randomdot stereograms. Optom Vis Sci. 2000;77(1):47-50.

4.
Parker AJ, Binocular depth perception and the cerebral cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8(5):379-391. crossref(new window)

5.
Qin D, Takamatsu M, Nakashima Y. Measurement for the panum's fusional area in retinal fovea using a threedimension display device. J Light & Vis Env. 2004;28(3): 126-131. crossref(new window)

6.
Gadia D, Garipoli G, Bonanomi C, Albani L, Rizzi A. Assessing stereo blindness and stereo acuity on digital displays. Displays. 2014;35(4):206-212. crossref(new window)

7.
Shin HS, Lee SH, Yun MO, Kim MY, Bae HS, Park SC. Relationship between the degree of exophoria and stereoacuity. J Korean Opthalmic Opt Soc. 2009;14(2):41-46.

8.
Lee WJ, Son JS, Kwak HW, Kim IS, Yu DS. Self-reported symptoms and stereopsis in viewing 2D and 3D images. J Korean Opthalmic Opt Soc. 2011;16(1):83-90.

9.
Kim DS, Lee WJ, Kim J, Yu DS, Jeong ET, Son JS. Change of phoria and subjective symptoms after watching 2D and 3D image. J Korean Opthalmic Opt Soc. 2012; 17(2):185-194.

10.
Choi JY, Kim JM, Kim HJ. Changes of stereoacuity with correction in induced anisometropia. J Korean Opthalmic Opt Soc. 2008;13(4):121-126.

11.
Westheimer G. Three-dimensional displays and stereo vision. Proc Biol Sci. 2011;278(1716):2241-2248. crossref(new window)

12.
Patterson R. Visual processing of depth information in stereoscopic displays. Displays. 1997;17(2):69-74. crossref(new window)

13.
Kim SH, Suh YW, Song JS, Park JH, Kim YY, Huh K et al. Clinical research on the ophthalmic factors affecting 3D asthenopia. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus. 2012; 49(4):248-253 crossref(new window)

14.
Frantz KA, Cotter SA, Brown WL, Motameni M. Erroneous findings in polarized testing caused by plastic prisms. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus. 1990;27(5):259-264.

15.
Shin-Nippon, Japan. PD METER PD82II. http://www.shin-nippon.jp/products/pd82/index.html(13 October 2015).

16.
Stereo Optical Company Incorporated, USA. Butterfly stereotest. http://www.stereooptical.com/shop/stereotests/butterfly-stereotest/(13 October 2015).

17.
Kang H, Hong H. Experimental determination of the range of binocular disparity for which stereoscopic fusion occurs at a viewing distance of 2.5m for a stereoscopic TV. J Soc Inf Dis. 2013;21(7):317-323. crossref(new window)

18.
LG. stereoscopic TV 47LW4000. http://www.lge.co.kr/lgekor/product/media/categoryMain.do(13 October 2015).

19.
Scheiman M, Wick B. Clinical Management of Binocular Vision: Heterophoric, Accommodative and Eye Movement Disorders. 3rd Ed. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2008;60-81.

20.
Saladin JJ. Stereopsis from a performance perspective. Optom Vis Sci. 2005;82(3):186-205. crossref(new window)

21.
Peli E. The visual effects of head-mounted display (HMD) are not distinguishable from those of desk-top computer display. Vision Res 1998;38(13):2053-2066. crossref(new window)

22.
Cho YA, Cho SW, Roh GH. Evaluation of criteria of Stereoacuity for Titmus, Randot & TNO Stereotests, J Korean Ophthalmol Soc. 1999;40(2):532-537.

23.
Westheimer G. Clinical evaluation of stereopsis. Vision Research. 2013;(90):38-42. crossref(new window)

24.
Westheimer G. The Ferrier Lecture, 1992. Seeing depth with two eyes: stereopsis. Proc Biol Society. 1994;257 (1349):205-214.

25.
McIntire JP, Havig PR, Geiselman EE. Stereoscopic 3D displays and human performance: A comprehensive review. Displays. 2014;35(1):18-26. crossref(new window)

26.
Froner B, Holliman NS, Liversedge SP, A comparative study of fine depth perception on two-view 3D displays. Displays. 2008;29(5):440-450. crossref(new window)

27.
SPSS Inc., Hong kong. PASW Statistics 18. http://www.spss.com.hk/statistics(13 October 2015).

28.
Kang SH, Hong HK. In watching 3D stereoscopic display using the binocular disparity, the effect of pupillary distance of adults and children on the perception of 3D Image. J Korean Ophthalmic Opt Soc. 2011;16(3):299-305.

29.
Hakkinen J, Takatalo J, Kilpelainen M, Salmimaa M, Nyman G. Determining limits to avoid double vision in an autostereoscopic display: Disparity and image element width. J Soc Inf Disp. 2009;(17):433-441. crossref(new window)

30.
Iwasaki T, Kubota T, Tawara A. The tolerance range of binocular disparity on a 3D display based on the physiological characteristics of ocular accommodation. Displays. 2009;30(1):44-48. crossref(new window)