References
- Alloway, P. (2007). Working memory, reading and mathematical skills in children with developmental coordination disorder. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 20-36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2006.07.002
- Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 829-839. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1201
- Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (pp. 47-90). New York: Academic Press.
- Barkley, A. (2012). Executive functions: What they are, how they work, and why they evolved. New York: Guilford Press.
- Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1-48.
- Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2013). Praat: doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 5.3.51. Retrieved from http://www.praat.org/ on August 15, 2017.
- Cebrian, J. (2006). Experience and the use of non-native duration in L2 vowel categorization. Journal of Phonetics, 34(3), 372-387. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2005.08.003
- Cedrus Corporation. (2015). SuperLab. Version 5.0.5. San Pedro, CA. [Computer program]
- Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450-466. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6
- Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual review of psychology, 64, 135-168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
- Eriksen, B., & Eriksen, C. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143-149. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03203267
- Escudero, P., & Boersma, P. (2004). Bridging the gap between L2 speech perception research and phonological theory. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 551-585.
- Flege, J., Bohn, O., & Jang, S. (1997). Effects of experience on non-native speakers' production and perception of English vowels. Journal of Phonetics, 25(4), 437-470. https://doi.org/10.1006/jpho.1997.0052
- Francis, A., Kaganovich, N., & Driscoll-Huber, C. (2008). Cue-specific effects of categorization training on the relative weighting of acoustic cues to consonant voicing in English. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124(2), 1234-1251. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2945161
- Friedman, N., Miyake, A., Corley, R., Young, S., DeFries, J., & Hewitt, J. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychological Science, 17(2), 172-179. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01681.x
- Gordon, P., Eberhardt, J., & Rueckl, J. (1993). Attentional modulation of the phonetic significance of acoustic cues. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 1-42. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1993.1001
- Hazan, V., & Barrett, S. (2000). The development of phonemic categorization in children aged 6-12. Journal of Phonetics, 28(4), 377-396. https://doi.org/10.1006/jpho.2000.0121
- Holt, L., & Lotto, A. (2006). Cue weighting in auditory categorization: Implications for first and second language acquisition. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119(5), 3059-3071. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2188377
- Hommel, B. (2011). The Simon effect as tool and heuristic. Acta Psychologica, 136, 189-202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.04.011
- Kang, Y. (2014). Voiceonset time merger and development of tonal contrast in Seoul Korean stop: Acorpus study. Journal of Phonetics, 45, 76-90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2014.03.005
- Kim, M. (2004). Correlation betweenVOT and F0 in the perception of Korean stops and affricates. Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Spoken Language Processing (pp. 49-52).
- Kim, S. (2015). ppcor: partial and semi-partial (part) Correlation. R package version 1.1. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ppcor on August 15, 2017.
- Kim, Y., Hong, G., Kim, K., Jang, H., & Lee, J. (2009). Receptive & expressive vocabulary test (REVT). Seoul: Seoul Community Rehabilitation Center.
- Kingston, J., & Diehl, R. (1994). Phonetic knowledge. Language, 419-454.
- Kong, E., & Edwards, J. (2016). Individual differences in categorical perception of speech: Cue weighting and executive function. Journal of Phonetics, 59, 40-57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2016.08.006
- Kong, E., & Kang, J. (2015). L2 proficiency and effect of auditory source in processing L2 stops. Phonetics and Speech Sciences, 7(3), 99-105. https://doi.org/10.13064/KSSS.2015.7.3.099
- Kong, E., & Kang, S. (2017). Individual variability in processing English source input into Korean stops. Studies in Phonetics, Phonology and Morphology, 23(2), 201-219. https://doi.org/10.17959/sppm.2017.23.2.201
- Kong, E., & Yoon, I. (2013). L2 proficiency effect on the acoustic cue-weighting pattern by Korean L2 learners of English: production and perception of English stops. Phonetics and Speech Sciences, 5(4), 81-90. https://doi.org/10.13064/KSSS.2013.5.4.081
- Kong, E., Beckman, M., & Edwards, J. (2011). Why are Korean tense stops acquired so early?: The role of acoustic properties. Journal of Phonetics, 39(2), 196-211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2011.02.002
- Kopp, B., Mattler, U., & Rist, F. (1994). Selective attention and response competition in schizophrenic patients. Psychiatry Research, 53(2), 129-139. https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-1781(94)90104-X
- Kronenberger, W., Pisoni, D., Henning, S., & Colson, B. (2013). Executive functioning skills in long-term users of cochlear implants: A case control study. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 38, 902-914. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jst034
- Lev-Ari, S., & Peperkamp, S. (2013). Low inhibitory skill leads to non-native perception and production in bilinguals' native language. Journal of Phonetics, 41, 320-331. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2013.06.002
- Lezak, M. (1983). Neuropsychological Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press.
- Li, F. (2012). Language specific developmental differences in speech production: A cross-language acoustic study. Child Development, 83(4), 1303-1315. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01773.x
- Lisker, L. (1978). Rapid vs. rabid: A catalogue of acoustic features that may cue the distinction. Haskins Laboratories Status Report on Speech Research, 54, 127-132.
- Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word, 20(3), 384-422. https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.1964.11659830
- Mayo, C., & Turk, A. (2004). Adult-child differences in acoustic cue weighting are influenced by segmental context: Children are not always perceptually biased toward transitions. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 115(6), 3184-3194. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1738838
- Mayr, R., & Escudero, P. (2010). Explaining individual variation in L2 perception: Rounded vowels in English learners of German. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(3), 279-297. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990022
- Miyake, A., Friedman, N., Emerson, M., Witzki, A., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "frontal lobe" tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49-100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
- Moon, S., & Byun, C. (1997). Korean Kaufman assessment battery for children (K-ABC). Seoul: Hakjisa.
- Nittrouer, S. (1992). Age-related differences in perceptual effects of formant transitions within syllables and across syllable boundaries. Journal of Phonetics, 20(3), 351-382.
- Nittrouer, S. (2002). Learning to perceive speech: How fricative perception changes, and how it stays the same. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 112(2), 711-719. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1496082
- Nittrouer, S., & Burton, L. (2005). The role of early language experience in the development of speech perception and phonological processing abilities: Evidence from 5-year-olds with histories of otitis media with effusion and low socioeconomic status. Journal of Communication Disorders, 38, 29-63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.03.006
- Nittrouer, S., & Miller, M. (1997). Developmental weighting shifts for noise components of fricative-vowel syllables. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102(1), 572-580. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.419730
- Nittrouer, S., Caldwell-Tarr, A., & Lowenstein, J. (2013). Working memory in children with cochlear implants: problems are in storage, not processing. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 77, 1886-1898. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2013.09.001
- Owen, A., McMillan, K., Laird, A., & Bullmore, E. (2005). N-back working memory paradigm: A meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 25(1), 46-59. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20131
- Psychology Software Tools, Incorporated. [E-Prime 2.0]. (2012). Retrieved from http://www.pstnet.com on August 15, 2017.
- R Core Team. (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/ on August 15, 2017.
- Repp, B. (1983). Coarticulation in sequences of two nonhomorganic stop consonants: perceptual and acoustic evidence. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 74(2), 420-427. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.389835
- Riki, T., Minka, H., & Liat, K. (2007). The effect of age on acoustic cue weighting in the perception of initial stop voicing contrast in Hebrew. Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences.
- Schertz, J., Cho, T., Lotto, A., & Warner, N. (2015). Individual differences in phonetic cue use in production and perception of a non-native sound contrast. Journal of Phonetics, 52, 183-204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2015.07.003
- Silva, D. (2006). Acoustic evidence for the emergence of tonal contrast in contemporaryKorean. Phonology, 23, 287-308. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952675706000911
- Stuss, D., Levine, B., Alexander, M., Hong, J., Palumbo, C., Hamer, L., Murphy, K., & Izukawa, D. (2000). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in patients with focal frontal and posterior brain damage: effects of lesion location and test structure on separable cognitive processes. Neuropsychologia, 38, 388-402. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00093-7
- Summerfield, Q., & Haggard, M. (1974). Perceptual processing of multiple cues and contexts: effects of following vowel upon stop consonant voicing. Journal of Phonetics, 2, 279-295.
- van Maanen, L., van Rijn, H., & Borst, J. (2009). Stroop and picture-word interference are two sides of the same coin. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(6), 987-999. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.6.987
- Vanderplas, J., & Garvin, E. (1959). Complexity, association value, and practice as factors in shape recognition following paired-associates training. Journal of experimental psychology, 57(3), 147-154. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048723
- Walley, A., & Carrell, T. (1983). Onset spectra and formant transitions in the adult's and child's perception of place of articulation in stop consonants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 73, 1011-1022. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.389149
- Yamada, R., & Tohkura, Y. (1992). The effects of experimental variables on the perception of American English /r/ and /l/ by Japanese listeners. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 52(4), 376-392. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206698
- Zelazo, P., Mueller, U., Frye, D., & Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68, 1-137. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.2003.00247.x