References
- Assadollahi, R., & Pulvermuller, F. (2001). Neuromagnetic evidence for early access to cognitive representations. Neuroreport, 12(2), 207-213. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200102120-00007
- Assadollahi, R., & Pulvermuller, F. (2003). Early influences of word length and frequency: A group study using MEG. Neuroreport, 14(8), 1183-1187. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200306110-00016
- Balota, D. A., & Chumbley, J. I. (1984). Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of word frequency in the neglected decision stage. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 10(3), 340-357. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.10.3.340
- Balota, D. A., & Chumbley, J. I. (1985). The locus of word-frequency effects in the pronunciation task: Lexical access and/or production? Journal of Memory and Language, 24(1), 89-106. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(85)90017-8
- Bell, A., Brenier, J. M., Gregory, M., Girand, C., & Jurafsky, D. (2009). Predictability effects on durations of content and function words in conversational English. Journal of Memory and Language, 60(1), 92-111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2008.06.003
- Choi, J. Y., Cho, H. S., & Nam, K. C. (2011). Effect of prosody on the phonological processing of Korean words. Communication Science & Disorders, 16(4), 614-626.
- Christophe, A., Peperkamp, S., Pallier, C., Block, E., & Mehler, J. (2004). Phonological phrase boundaries constrain lexical access I. Adult data. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(4), 523-547. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.07.001
- Connine, C. M., Titone, D., & Wang, J. (1993). Auditory word recognition: Extrinsic and intrinsic effects of word frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(1), 81-94. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.19.1.81
- Cutler, A., & Foss, D. J. (1977). On the role of sentence stress in sentence processing. Language and Speech, 20(1), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1177/002383097702000101
- Cutler, A., & Norris, D. (1988). The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 14(1), 113-121. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.14.1.113
- Dahan, D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2001). Time course of frequency effects in spoken-word recognition: Evidence from eye movements. Cognitive Psychology, 42(4), 317-367. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.2001.0750
- Foster, K. I. (1976). Accessing the mental lexicon. New Approaches to Language Mechanisms, 257-287.
- Frazier, L., & Fodor, J. D. (1978). The sausage machine: A new two-stage parsing model. Cognition, 6(4), 291-325. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(78)90002-1
- Gout, A., Christophe, A., & Morgan, J. L. (2004). Phonological phrase boundaries constrain lexical access II. Infant data. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(4), 548-567. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.07.002
- Hauk, O., & Pulvermuller, F. (2004). Effects of word length and frequency on the human event-related potential. Clinical Neurophysiology, 155(5), 1090-1103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2003.12.020
- Hauk, O., Davis, M. H., Ford, M., Pulvermuller, F., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2006). The time course of visual word recognition as revealed by linear regression analysis of ERP data. Neuroimage, 30(4), 1383-1400. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.048
- Jescheniak, J. D., & Levelt, W. J. (1994). Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4), 824. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.20.4.824
- Jun, S. A. (2000). K-ToBI (Korean ToBI) labelling conventions. Version 3.1. Retrieved from https://linguistics.ucla.edu/people/jun/ktobi/k-tobi.html
- Jusczyk, P. W., Houston, D. M., & Newsome, M. (1999). The beginnings of word segmentation in English-learning infants. Cognitive Psychology, 39(3-4), 159-207. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0716
- Kim, S., & Cho, T. (2009). The use of phrase-level prosodic information in lexical segmentation: Evidence from word-spotting experiments in Korean. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 125(5), 3373-3386. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3097777
- Lee, S. H., Shin, J., Kim, B. Y., & Lee, Y. J. (2003). Some considerations on SiTEC segmental and prosodic labeling convention for Korean. Malsori, 46, 127-143.
- Mannel, C., & Friederici, A. D. (2009). Pauses and intonational phrasing: ERP studies in 5-month-old German infants and adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(10), 1988-2006. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21221
- McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18(1), 1-86. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(86)90015-0
- McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88(5), 375-407. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.88.5.375
- Monsell, S. (2012). The nature and locus of word frequency effects in reading. In Basic processes in reading (pp. 148-197). London:Routledge.
- Monsell, S., Doyle, M. C., & Haggard, P. N. (1989). Effects of frequency on visual word recognition tasks: Where are they? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118(1), 43-71. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.118.1.43
- Morton, J. (1969). Interaction of information in word recognition. Psychological Review, 76(2), 165. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0027366
- Nan, Y., Knosche, T. R., & Friederici, A. D. (2009). Non-musicians' perception of phrase boundaries in music: A cross-cultural ERP study. Biological Psychology, 82(1), 70-81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.06.002
- Nazzi, T., Iakimova, G., Bertoncini, J., Fredonie, S., & Alcantara, C. (2006). Early segmentation of fluent speech by infants acquiring French: Emerging evidence for crosslinguistic differences. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(3), 283-299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.10.004
- Polich, J., & Donchin, E. (1988). P300 and the word frequency effect. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 70(1), 33-45. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(88)90192-7
- Ralston, J. V., Pisoni, D. B., Lively, S. E., Greene, B. G., & Mullennix, J. W. (1991). Comprehension of synthetic speech produced by rule: Word monitoring and sentence-by-sentence listening times. Human Factors, 33(4), 471-491. https://doi.org/10.1177/001872089103300408
- Rubenstein, H., Garfield, L., & Millikan, J. A. (1970). Homographic entries in the internal lexicon. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9(5), 487-494. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(70)80091-3
- Rugg, M. D. (1990). Event-related brain potentials dissociate repetition effecs of high-and low-frequency words. Memory and Cognition, 18(4), 367-379. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197126
- Schvaneveldt, R. W., & McDonald, J. E. (1981). Semantic context and the encoding of words: Evidence for two modes of stimulus analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7(3), 673-687. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.7.3.673
- Sereno, S. C., Rayner, K., & Posner, M. I. (1998). Establishing a time-line for word recognition: Evidence from eye movements and event-related potentials. Neuro Report, 9(10), 2195-2200.
- Shields, J. L., McHugh, A., & Martin, J. G. (1974). Reaction time to phoneme targets as a function of rhythmic cues in continuous speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102(2), 250-255. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0035855
- Shin, J. (2011). Korean phonetics and phology. Seoul: Pagijong.
- Smith, M., & Halgren, E. (1987). Event-related potentials during lexical decision: Effects of repetition, word frequency, pronouncebaility, and concreteness. Electroencephalography Clinical Neurophysioogy, 40, 417-421.
- Tyler, L. K. (1989). Syntactic deficits and the construction of local phrases in spoken language comprehension. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 6(3), 333-355. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643298908253423
- Tyler, L. K., & Warren, P. (1987). Local and global structure in spoken language comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 26(6), 638-657. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(87)90107-0
- Van Petten, C., & Kutas, M. (1990). Interaction between sentence context and word frequency in event-related brain potentials. Memory and Cognition, 18(4), 380-393. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197127
- Zhao, J., Guo, J., Zhou, F., & Shu, H. (2011). Time course of Chinese monosyllabic spoken word recognition: Evidence from ERP analyses. Neuropsychologia, 49(7), 1761-1770. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.054