Voice onset time in English and Korean stops with respect to a sound change

  • Kim, Mi-Ryoung (Department of Practical Foreign Languages, Korea Soongsil Cyber University)
  • Received : 2021.05.30
  • Accepted : 2021.06.18
  • Published : 2021.06.30

Abstract

Voice onset time (VOT) is known to be a primary acoustic cue that differentiates voiced from voiceless stops in the world's languages. While much attention has been given to the sound change of Korean stops, little attention has been given to that of English stops. This study examines VOT of stop consonants as produced by English speakers in comparison to Korean speakers to see whether there is any VOT change for English stops and how the effects of stop, place, gender, and individual on VOT differ cross-linguistically. A total of 24 native speakers (11 Americans and 13 Koreans) participated in this experiment. The results showed that, for Korean, the VOT merger of lax and aspirated stops was replicated, and, for English, voiced stops became initially devoiced and voiceless stops became heavily aspirated. English voiceless stops became longer in VOT than Korean counterparts. The results suggest that, similar to Korean stops, English stops may also undergo a sound change. Since it is the first study to be revealed, more convincing evidence is necessary.

Keywords

Acknowledgement

I am very grateful to three anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful comments. I'd like to thank all participants.

References

  1. Abramson, A. S., & Lisker, L. (1970, January). Discriminability along the voicing continuum: Cross-language tests. Proceedings of the Sixth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 569-573). Prague, Czech.
  2. Abramson, A. S., & Whalen, D. H. (2017). Voice Onset Time (VOT) at 50: Theoretical and practical issues in measuring voicing distinctions. Journal of Phonetics, 63, 75-86. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2017.05.002
  3. Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2018). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (version 6.0.37) [Computer program]. Retrieved from http://www.praat.org/
  4. Caramazza, A., Yeni-Komshian, G. H., Zurif, E. B., & Carbone, E. (1973). The acquisition of a new phonological contrast: The case of stop consonants in French-English bilinguals. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 54(2), 421-428. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1913594
  5. Chodroff, E., & Wilson, C. (2017). Structure in talker-specific phonetic realization: Covariation of stop consonant VOT in American English. Journal of Phonetics, 61, 30-47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2017.01.001
  6. Docherty, G. L. (1992). The timing of voicing in British English obstruents. New York, NY: Foris.
  7. Flege, J. E. (1982). Laryngeal timing and phonation onset in utterance-initial English stops. Journal of Phonetics, 10(2), 177-192. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0095-4470(19)30956-8
  8. Fowler, C. A., Sramko V., Ostry, D. J., Rowland, S. A., & Halle, P. (2008). Cross language phonetic influences on the speech of French-English bilinguals. Journal of Phonetics, 36(4), 649-663. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2008.04.001
  9. Haggard, M., Ambler, S., & Callow, M. (1970). Pitch as a voicing cue. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 47(2B), 613-617. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1911936
  10. Han, M. S., & Weitzman, R. S. (1970). Acoustic features of Korean /P, T, K/, /p, t, k/ and /ph, th, kh/. Phonetica, 22(2), 112-128. https://doi.org/10.1159/000259311
  11. Kang, Y. (2014). Voice onset time merger and development of tonal contrast in Seoul Korean stops: A corpus study. Journal of Phonetics, 45, 76-90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2014.03.005
  12. Keating, P. A. (1984). Phonetic and phonological representation of stop consonant voicing. Language, 60(2), 286-319. https://doi.org/10.2307/413642
  13. Kim, C. W. (1965). On the autonomy of the tensity feature in stop classification (with special reference to Korean stops). Word, 21(3), 339-359. https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.1965.11435434
  14. Kim, M. R. C. (1994). Acoustic characteristics of Korean stops and perception of English stop consonants (Doctoral dissertation). University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI.
  15. Kim, M. -R. (2000). Segmental and tonal interactions in English and Korean: A phonetic and phonological study (Doctoral dissertation). The University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI.
  16. Kim, M. -R. (2008). Lax stops in Korean revisited: VOT neutralization. Studies in Phonetics, Phonology and Morphology, 14(2), 3-20.
  17. Kim, M.-R. (2011). Native and non-native English speakers' VOT productions of stops. The Linguistic Association of Korea Journal, 19(1), 97-116.
  18. Kim, M. -R. (2014). Ongoing sound change in the stop system of Korean: A three- to two-way categorization. Studies in Phonetics, Phonology, and Morphology, 20(1), 51-82. https://doi.org/10.17959/SPPM.2014.20.1.51
  19. Kim, M. -R. (2020). The effects of length of residence (LOR) on voice onset time (VOT). Phonetics and Speech Sciences, 12(4), 9-17.
  20. Kim, M. -R., Beddor, P. S., & Horrocks, J. (2002). The contribution of consonantal and vocalic information to the perception of Korean initial stops. Journal of Phonetics, 30(1), 77-100. https://doi.org/10.1006/jpho.2001.0152
  21. Klatt, D. H. (1975). Voice onset time, frication, and aspiration in word-initial consonant clusters. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 18(4), 686-706. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.1804.686
  22. Kohler, K. J. (1982). F0 in the production of fortis and lenis plosives. Phonetica, 39(4-5), 199-218. https://doi.org/10.1159/000261663
  23. Kong, E. J., Beckman, M. E., & Edwards, J. (2012). Voice onset time is necessary but not always sufficient to describe acquisition of voiced stops: The cases of Greek and Japanese. Journal of Phonetics, 40(6), 725-744. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2012.07.002
  24. Ladefoged, P., & Maddieson, I. (1996). The sounds of the world's languages. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
  25. Liberman, A. M., Delattre, P. C., & Cooper, F. S. (1958). Some cues for the distinction between voiced and voiceless stops in initial position. Language and Speech, 1(3), 153-167. https://doi.org/10.1177/002383095800100301
  26. Lieberman, P., & Blumstein, S. E. (1988). Speech physiology, speech perception, and acoustic phonetics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  27. Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word, 20(3), 384-422. https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.1964.11659830
  28. Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1967). Some effects of context on voice onset time in English stops. Language and Speech, 10(1), 1-28. https://doi.org/10.1177/002383096701000101
  29. Nakai, S., & Scobbie, J. M. (2016). The VOT category boundary in word-initial stops: Counter-evidence against rate normalization in English spontaneous speech. Laboratory Phonology: Journal of the Association for Laboratory Phonology, 7(1), 1-31. https://doi.org/10.5334/labphon.7
  30. Rothenberg, M. (2009). Voice onset time versus articulatory modeling for stop consonants. Logopedics Phoniatrics Vocology, 34(4), 171-180. https://doi.org/10.3109/14015430903002270
  31. Silva, D. J. (2006). Acoustic evidence for the emergence of tonal contrast in contemporary Korean. Phonology, 23(2), 287-308. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952675706000911
  32. SPSS/PASW statistics. (2017). IBM SPSS statistics for Windows. Version 25.0. Armonk, NY: IBM.
  33. Whalen, D. H., Abramson, A. S., Lisker, L., & Mody, M. (1993). F0 gives voicing information even with unambiguous voice onset times. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 93(4), 2152-2159. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.406678
  34. Winn, M. B., Chatterjee, M., & Idsardi, W. J. (2013). Roles of voice onset time and F0 in stop consonant voicing perception: Effects of masking noise and low-pass filtering. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(4), 1097-1107. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/12-0086)